Show simple item record

dc.contributor.authorZerbini, Giulia
dc.contributor.authorvan der Vinne, Vincent
dc.contributor.authorOtto, Lana K. M.
dc.contributor.authorKantermann, Thomas
dc.contributor.authorKrijnen, Wim P.
dc.contributor.authorRoenneberg, Till
dc.contributor.authorMerrow, Martha
dc.date2022-08-11T08:09:48.000
dc.date.accessioned2022-08-23T16:43:47Z
dc.date.available2022-08-23T16:43:47Z
dc.date.issued2017-06-29
dc.date.submitted2017-11-01
dc.identifier.citationSci Rep. 2017 Jun 29;7(1):4385. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-04076-y. <a href="https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-04076-y">Link to article on publisher's site</a>
dc.identifier.issn2045-2322 (Linking)
dc.identifier.doi10.1038/s41598-017-04076-y
dc.identifier.pmid28663569
dc.identifier.urihttp://hdl.handle.net/20.500.14038/40369
dc.description.abstractSuccess at school determines future career opportunities. We described a time-of-day specific disparity in school performance between early and late chronotypes. Several studies showed that students with a late chronotype and short sleep duration obtain lower grades, suggesting that early school starting times handicap their performance. How chronotype, sleep duration, and time of day impact school performance is not clear. At a Dutch high school, we collected 40,890 grades obtained in a variety of school subjects over an entire school year. We found that the strength of the effect of chronotype on grades was similar to that of absenteeism, and that late chronotypes were more often absent. The difference in grades between the earliest 20% and the latest 20% of chronotypes corresponds to a drop from the 55th to 43rd percentile of grades. In academic subjects using mainly fluid cognition (scientific subjects), the correlation with grades and chronotype was significant while subjects relying on crystallised intelligence (humanistic/linguistic) showed no correlation with chronotype. Based on these and previous results, we can expand our earlier findings concerning exam times: students with a late chronotype are at a disadvantage in exams on scientific subjects, and when they are examined early in the day.
dc.language.isoen_US
dc.relation<p><a href="http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/entrez/query.fcgi?db=pubmed&cmd=Retrieve&list_uids=28663569&dopt=Abstract">Link to Article in PubMed</a></p>
dc.rights© The Author(s) 2017
dc.rights.urihttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/
dc.subjectEducation
dc.subjectNeuroscience and Neurobiology
dc.titleLower school performance in late chronotypes: underlying factors and mechanisms
dc.typeJournal Article
dc.source.journaltitleScientific reports
dc.source.volume7
dc.source.issue1
dc.identifier.legacyfulltexthttps://escholarship.umassmed.edu/cgi/viewcontent.cgi?article=4176&amp;context=oapubs&amp;unstamped=1
dc.identifier.legacycoverpagehttps://escholarship.umassmed.edu/oapubs/3168
dc.identifier.contextkey10981251
refterms.dateFOA2022-08-23T16:43:47Z
html.description.abstract<p>Success at school determines future career opportunities. We described a time-of-day specific disparity in school performance between early and late chronotypes. Several studies showed that students with a late chronotype and short sleep duration obtain lower grades, suggesting that early school starting times handicap their performance. How chronotype, sleep duration, and time of day impact school performance is not clear. At a Dutch high school, we collected 40,890 grades obtained in a variety of school subjects over an entire school year. We found that the strength of the effect of chronotype on grades was similar to that of absenteeism, and that late chronotypes were more often absent. The difference in grades between the earliest 20% and the latest 20% of chronotypes corresponds to a drop from the 55th to 43rd percentile of grades. In academic subjects using mainly fluid cognition (scientific subjects), the correlation with grades and chronotype was significant while subjects relying on crystallised intelligence (humanistic/linguistic) showed no correlation with chronotype. Based on these and previous results, we can expand our earlier findings concerning exam times: students with a late chronotype are at a disadvantage in exams on scientific subjects, and when they are examined early in the day.</p>
dc.identifier.submissionpathoapubs/3168
dc.contributor.departmentNeurobiology
dc.source.pages4385


Files in this item

Thumbnail
Name:
s41598_017_04076_y.pdf
Size:
1.156Mb
Format:
PDF

This item appears in the following Collection(s)

Show simple item record

© The Author(s) 2017
Except where otherwise noted, this item's license is described as © The Author(s) 2017